Okay, thank you very much. I apologize for asking you to repeat it. I was concentrating so much on the second part of your question, I forgot the first part.
Regarding the new scientific research, there haven't been any studies published recently, and that's because when you're studying populations and looking at these effects, you look at them over their lifetime. Then you might give occasional progress updates. For example, people are still studying the victims from Nagasaki and Hiroshima, the atomic bomb survivors.
So, you follow a cohort of people from the time of exposure to the time they die, and when the cohort has completely passed away, you can make a conclusive statement on whether or not you have any adverse effects.
In that respect, the studies that are the most important are the studies of workers, which were undertaken in the forties and fifties in Britain and the U.S.A., because there were a large number of people in those studies. If you have small studies, then they're subject to significant error. This is just a consequence of small numbers. If you toss a coin 1,000 times, it's going to be closer to a one-to-one ratio for heads and tails than it is if you toss a coin three or four times. So there is an inevitable concentration on the larger studies, because those are deemed to be the most powerful. Studies of people from a while ago—a lot of whom have died, and so we have a good history on those individuals—are the most powerful ones as well.
That is why I think it's justified to do that. I think it's very important to realize though that these individuals, these people, who claim to be damaged have real problems. Something caused those problems, and it's important to find out what caused those problems. I don't believe it was uranium.